Mobile Menu Open Mobile Menu Close

Search by:



Population and breeding census of New Zealand king shag (Leucocarbo carunculatus) in 2015

Notornis, 62 (4), 209-218

R. Schuckard; D.S. Melville; G. Taylor (2015)

Article Type: Paper

Prior to 1992 the total population of New Zealand king shag (Leucocarbo carunculatus) was estimated to be about 300 individuals. Between 1992 and 2002, colonies in the outer Marlborough Sounds, New Zealand were surveyed by boat and the total population was estimated to be 645 birds. About 92% of all birds occurred at Duffers Reef, North Trio Island, Sentinel Rock, and White Rocks, with an estimated 102-126 breeding pairs. A survey in February 2015 was the first to be conducted from the air. All colonies were photographed within 44 minutes prior to the morning departure and the total population was estimated to be 839 individuals. A total of 187 pairs/nests were recorded using aerial 3D images of all breeding colonies in June 2015. North Trio Island was the largest breeding colony with 33.7% of all nests, followed by Duffers Reef with 18.7% of all nests. Despite the larger revised population size, the species remains Nationally Endangered.














Foraging behaviour and habitat partitioning in sympatric invasive birds in French Polynesia

Notornis, 61 (1), 35-42

J.H. Bates; E.N. Spotswood; J.C. Russell (2014)

Article Type: Paper

Interactions among invasive bird species have received relatively little attention despite the potential important consequences for community dynamics and invasion spread. Where species occupy similar environments the outcome may be particularly unpredictable. In this study we examined the foraging behaviours of 2 sympatric invasive birds in Mo’orea, the common myna (Acridotheres tristis) and the red-vented bulbul (Pycnonotus cafer), to determine if they exhibit behavioural or habitat partitioning. We investigated the ratio of foraging to vigilance behaviour of both species at novel food stations, as well as the effects of foraging group size and species composition on their behaviour. While the myna and bulbul exhibit similar behaviour, sites where they occur were partitioned within habitats. Novel food stations however, encouraged foraging at the cost of vigilance behaviours. Behaviours were not influenced by conspecific group size, and intraspecific interactions were more frequent, and of a greater intensity than interspecific interactions. These results suggest that the 2 species minimise competitive interactions by occupying different sites within the same habitat, and prioritise food acquisition in a new environment. Sympatric introduced species may avoid competitive exclusion if habitat usage limits the frequency of interactions.




Calling and display by peacocks (Pavo cristatus) at Mansion House Historic Reserve, Kawau Island, New Zealand

Notornis, 61 (1), 27-34

A.J. Beauchamp (2014)

Article Type: Paper

Information was collected on the calling and display behaviour of 3 peacocks in the absence of peahens at Mansion House Historic Reserve, Kawau Island. The peacocks used an exploded lek display structure from late August to late December. Peacocks predominantly used the eow and occasionally the ka notes during tail displays but were silent during many displays. The overall composition of call notes given during the breeding period differed from than those used in a large dense peafowl breeding population in Japan.